2015年8月23日 星期日

CMU影片迎2019級新生

 CMU影片迎2019級新生



這所大學,CMU (聽說Google公司為求才,在校外成立約200人的辦公室),我沒去過,雖然我東海同學嚴匡漢及其子都是該校校友。我與大學者Herbert A. Simon 的一些通信存在該校圖書館,影片有他跟其摯友的合照。李開復也在其中。類似的片子和做法,實在很值得各校觀摩。


1,564 次觀看
Carnegie Mellon University 上傳了一個新的影片
Welcome to campus, Class of 2019! Among you could be Broadway's biggest stars, Nobel Prize winners, political leaders and more. Good luck, and we can't wait to add you to this list!

2015年8月19日 星期三

Pittsburgh redevelopment

 Simon的著作如人工科學等,多提過該市的區域再生。這 Pittsburgh redevelopment規劃案,應該是持續的工程。


UDream offers students hands-on experience in urban design. One student's experience led to a successful bid on a Pittsburgh redevelopment plan.

Ashley Cox (A 2017) wanted to bring her dreams into focus. After...
CMU.LI |由CARNEGIE MELLON UNIVERSITY上傳

2015年8月18日 星期二

NEW INFORMATION IS EASIER TO LEARN WHEN COMPOSED OF FAMILIAR ELEMENTS

Thursday, August 13, 2015

NEW INFORMATION IS EASIER TO LEARN WHEN COMPOSED OF FAMILIAR ELEMENTS

CMU Psychologists Uncover Critical Relationship Between Working Memory and the Strength of Information “Chunks”

By Shilo Rea / 412-268-6094 / shilo@cmu.edu
brain
People have more difficulty recalling the string of letters BIC, IAJ, FKI, RSU and SAF than FBI, CIA, JFK, IRS and USA. The well-established reason is that the amount of information we can hold in our short-term or working memory is affected by whether the information can be “chunked” into larger units.
New research by Carnegie Mellon University psychologists takes this learning principle one step further by uncovering how the strength — or familiarity — of those chunks plays a crucial role. Published in Psychonomic Bulletin Review, they show for the first time that it is easier to learn new facts that are composed of more familiar chunks.
These findings have implications for how students are taught almost any subject, including second language learning. They also indicate that the long-held belief that children have less working memory than adults may not be true because working memory resources are more rapidly consumed when the chunks are less familiar.
This novel research merges instructional innovation and brain science, two of Carnegie Mellon’s university-wide initiatives — the Simon Initiative, which aims to measurably improve student learning outcomes by harnessing decades of learning science research, and BrainHub, which focuses on how the structure and activity of the brain give rise to complex behaviors.
“We are suggesting that working memory capacity is not a fixed quantity but interacts with the familiarity of the elements that need to be processed. If everything is very familiar, it is easy to comprehend and build new knowledge. If all of the components are unfamiliar, the task becomes very difficult or impossible,” said Lynne Reder, professor of psychology in the Dietrich College of Humanities and Social Sciences and a leading expert on memory, cognition and behavior. Reder is also a member of CMU’s Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition (CNBC) and the Human-Computer Interaction Institute.
For the study, 20 CMU undergraduates with no prior experience with Chinese language learning were familiarized with 64 Chinese characters during three-hour long sessions each week for one month. This involved searching for a particular character in visual display of similar characters. (See top row of Figure 1.) Half of the characters were randomly selected to display at a higher frequency — 20 times more often — to become more familiar to the participants, leaving the remaining “low frequency” characters less familiar.
memory and learning
Figure 1.
Each week, participants also completed a memory test requiring them to learn an association between a pair of characters and an arbitrary English word. (See the second row in Figure 1.) Each pair of characters was completely new and was composed either of the low-frequency or the high-frequency characters. The researchers found that novel combinations of characters that were made from the more familiar characters were easier to learn to associate with new information than combinations made from the less familiar characters.
Reder and her team theorized that learning would be faster and more accurate with more familiar characters because, according to them, stronger chunks consume less working memory and working memory is critical for forming new knowledge. To further support for the finding that more familiar chunks consume less working memory resources, they had participants complete a popular test of working-memory (N-back) with both the high- and low-frequency characters and found that the N-back task was much easier with the high frequency characters. 
“This work has implications for how to optimize instruction, specifically that concepts should be introduced to students in a way that they have a good grasp and familiarity with those concepts before trying to combine them into more complex informational structures. These findings may also help to explain certain paradoxes such as why children tend to learn computer applications more easily than adults and may help to explain why they learn second languages better than adults,” Reder said. “Little kids may actually have more working memory than adults. They often appear to have less only because they have fewer knowledge chunks and those chunks are weaker than adults'.” 
Reder continued, “Adults have wisdom — knowledge and skills — and scientists have been confusing that with greater working memory.”
In addition to Reder, the research team included CMU’s Xiaonan Liu, Alexander Keinath and Vencislav Popov. This study was partially supported by the National Institute of Mental Health.